Los Angeles Times

The psychology behind taking dirty money from Jeffrey Epstein

- Nina Strohminge­r is a professor of business ethics and legal studies at the Wharton School and a professor of psychology at the University of Pennsylvan­ia. By Nina Strohminge­r

Why do good people take bad money? And what are the insidious effects of doing so?

These are puzzles surroundin­g Jeffrey Epstein, the convicted sex offender, who so easily insinuated himself into prestigiou­s universiti­es and an orbit of prominent scientists years after his 2008 conviction. His billion-dollar fortune allowed him to lavishly fund their research activities and centers, among them MIT’s Media Lab.

But those receiving the money knew about his past. In fact, senior leaders at MIT approved Epstein’s donations and decided to keep them anonymous. MIT’s president, L. Rafael Reif, even signed a letter in 2012 thanking Epstein for a gift. Joi Ito has since resigned as the director of the MIT Media Lab, and others have apologized, but only after the gifts became public.

How could these educationa­l leaders have taken Epstein’s money, knowing it was so tainted that the gifts needed to remain anonymous?

On a certain accounting, money is money. And isn’t the greatest good maximized when cash is directed out of the pockets of unsavory characters and into worthy research?

This calculus may have the patina of logic, but it is wrong. Consider the psychologi­cal principles at play when people take money from wrongdoers.

In a series of recent experiment­s, Arber Tasimi and others have shown that morally tainted money is worth less than face value. People would rather have $99 from a good guy than $100 from a bad guy.

However, when the differenti­al becomes big enough, people abandon their principles, selecting $100 from a bad guy over $1 from a good guy. Even babies show this pattern, forgoing two crackers from an evil puppet in favor of a single cracker from a nice puppet. But when the evil puppet has eight crackers, they cave.

People have a deep-seated aversion to morally tainted money but also a strong temptation for the reward. When people take dirty money, this inner conflict is resolved with rationaliz­ation. And the power of rationaliz­ation is so strong that people will engage in elaborate cognitive gymnastics not to see the negative ramificati­ons of what they have just done. Our behavior will shape-shift to protect our benefactor.

Research on conflicts of interest shows that, when confronted with the possibilit­y that their benefactor is morally dubious, people will look the other way, selectivel­y examine the evidence in front of them, and generally focus on whatever slim evidence remains that their benefactor has redeeming qualities. As Upton Sinclair observed, it is difficult to get a man to understand something when his salary depends upon his not understand­ing it.

The power of Epstein’s money had the intended effect on the scientists in his circle. Some, such as Robert Trivers and Lawrence Krauss, have defended him. Many others have remained silent, and this silence serves as a tacit endorsemen­t. A large body of research in social psychology indicates that people infer what is acceptable based on the actions of others, particular­ly those with status. Mere associatio­n with Epstein is a powerful social signal that minimizes his alleged crimes. After all, how bad could he be if so many respected scientists rubbed elbows with him?

In effect, this process amounts to a kind of moral money laundering. By accepting Epstein’s gifts, these scientists and institutio­ns bestowed upon him their unspoken approval. Just as there is guilt by associatio­n with the corrupt, there is grace by associatio­n with the good (and in this case, the prestigiou­s). Coffers lined with dirty money dry up eventually, but the erosion of trust in research it causes is lasting.

The Epstein story may be unusually lurid, but it reveals an underlying systemic problem that will continue to plague our institutio­ns if change does not occur. Ito is gone from the MIT Media Lab, but most people (including those who continue to raise money at research institutio­ns) share his vulnerabil­ities.

This problem cannot simply be solved by hiring better people. Policies, incentives and norms must be put in place that combat the psychologi­cal weaknesses introduced by dirty money. Just as the best way to stick to a diet is to keep cake out of sight, the best way to make unbiased decisions is to reduce access to informatio­n that creates bias.

Universiti­es and research centers could, for example, evaluate gifts by reviewing the donor only and being blind to the size of the donation. Only through structural changes can we make our institutio­ns less vulnerable to those who would use them to launder their reputation­s.

Epstein was a predator in more ways than one. If we do not take steps to fortify our institutio­ns, the wolves will creep back in.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States