Los Angeles Times

What is driving bogus pedophilia claims?

Studies show that conspiracy theories about child predation exert a stronger hold among conservati­ves.

- By Melissa Healy

In modern presidenti­al campaigns, unsubstant­iated conspiracy theories about pedophilia have become almost as routine as stump speeches, poll results and televised debates.

In 2016, Democrats’ imagined penchant for child predation was manifested in the made- up charge that Hillary Clinton presided over a child sex- traffickin­g ring. Adherents of this f iction spun an elaborate online fantasy that the presidenti­al candidate — along with her husband, former President Clinton, and other prominent Democrats — was running such an enterprise from a nonexisten­t network of tunnels beneath a real- life pizza joint in Washington.

Clinton lost the election, but the fabricated charge that a secret cabal of pedophiles rules the Democratic Party remained a central feature of the QAnon conspiracy theory. Its adherents are allied with President Trump, the man they see as leading the charge against these “deep state” elites.

Now similar f ictions are being aimed at Joe Biden, the current Democratic presidenti­al nominee.

The hashtag # PedoBiden has been on the rise in recent months, and Trump gave it a boost by retweeting a video from an anonymous Twitter account that purported to show Biden in the act. The issue came up in Trump’s Thursday night town hall, when the president refused to repudiate QAnon and commended its adherents for being “strongly against pedophilia.”

In fact, the woman shown with Biden’s hands on her shoulders and his mouth to her ear was in her 40s at the time, and she has denounced the smear campaign against the former vice president. But Trump’s elevation of the false pedophilia claim ensures its place in the f irmament of conspirato­rial fabricatio­ns.

It also raises questions. Why do conspiracy theories built around pedophilia hold sway for so many? And why do their adherents tend to favor Trump?

Of all the epithets hurled by politician­s through history, an accusation of child sexual abuse strikes particular­ly deep chords, experts say.

Those chords resonate across the political spectrum. They rivet attention by evoking vulnerable children. They engage one of humankind’s most primitive and powerful emotions: disgust. And they place members of this evil cabal in a class beyond redemption.

Like all conspiracy theories with legs, this one draws credibilit­y from evil deeds we know to be real. After all, the cases against Catholic clergy, the f inancier Jeffrey Epstein and the disgraced Olympic gymnastics team doctor Larry Nassar all suggest that child sexual abuse can be perpetrate­d by fellow citizens we may once have trusted and admired.

But some researcher­s have gathered evidence that conspiracy theories involving pedophilia tend to exert a stronger and more enduring hold on people who identify as conservati­ve or embrace policy positions strongly identified with American conservati­sm.

In an inf luential 1964 article, Columbia University political scientist Richard Hofstadter described a particular strain of American partisans who employed “heated exaggerati­on, suspicious­ness and conspirato­rial fantasy” in their discourse. These people — a group typified by Sen. Joseph McCarthy, who initiated a range of investigat­ions to root out suspected communists in the federal government — “believe themselves to be conservati­ves and usually employ the rhetoric of conservati­sm,” he wrote.

Although Hofstadter’s thesis came in for criticism, it spurred research. Over the next 44 years, 88 studies conducted in 12 countries and involving more than 22,000 participan­ts drew links between people who embrace conservati­ve ideologies and attributes such as death anxiety, intoleranc­e of ambiguity and a personal need for order, structure and closure.

And f indings published in 2020 offer further evidence that this fits with contempora­ry politics in the United States, the study’s authors say.

In four separate experiment­al studies using various measures of political ideology and openness to conspiracy belief, a team of social psychologi­sts and political scientists led by Sander van der Linden of Cambridge University in England found that conservati­ves in the United States were “significan­tly and substantia­lly more likely than liberals to embrace conspirato­rial ways of thinking.”

Drawing from nationally representa­tive survey samples involving 2,500 Americans, the researcher­s found a relationsh­ip between conservati­sm and conspirato­rial thinking that was “positive, linear and statistica­lly robust,” they reported in the journal Political Psychology.

Although some research suggests that extreme liberals are just as prone to conspiracy belief as extreme conservati­ves, Van der Linden and his colleagues did not observe this. In both their large samples and in smaller groups designed to retest their f indings, they found that conspiraci­es were more readily embraced by people on the far right than by those on the far left.

Against this backdrop, conspiracy theories that involve pedophilia offer “kind of a perfect storm” of circumstan­ces to foster these misguided beliefs, according to UCLA evolutiona­ry anthropolo­gist Daniel M. T. Fessler, who wasn’t involved in the Political Psychology study.

For starters, we are primed by our mammalian brains to give credence to such charges, said Fessler, whose research focuses on the conditions in which conspiraci­es thrive. As a species that gives birth to helpless offspring and socializes our young over many years, humans respond to threats to a child’s well- being with fierce arousal.

In two experiment­s that recruited nearly 600 parents of young children, Fessler has shown that both parenthood and the presence of a young child enhance our distress when we perceive a threat to a child.

Since we depend on the group to protect our young, we have all evolved to respond to the threat of a child’s harm with horror and disgust, he said.

And that disgust is a powerful teacher. The putrid scent of spoiled foods and the overt signs of disease probably elicit feelings of disgust to ensure that we avoid the dangers of poisoning or contagion — it is a “hallmark emotion of fear,” Fessler said. Researcher­s have found that urban legends with a “yuck” factor are more likely to be remembered and passed on than those without.

At the same time, neuropsych­ological studies have demonstrat­ed that the brain’s responses to moral and physical disgust strongly overlap. It’s a sign that humans mark one another’s behavioral transgress­ions as evidence of danger and that we’ll keep a safe distance from the people who engage in them.

In his tweets, Trump routinely expresses disgust at policies he opposes, the people who espouse those policies or the places from which his perceived adversarie­s come. Since taking office in 2017, the president’s Twitter offerings have included the words “disgust,” “disgusted” or “disgusting ” at least 54 times, often accompanie­d by words such as “sick,” “vile,” “dangerous” and “filthy.”

Either deliberate­ly or subconscio­usly, Trump’s rhetoric also capitalize­s on another human inclinatio­n: In general, we believe and remember bad things more than neutral or pleasant ones.

This is the natural result of the evolutiona­ry principle known to psychologi­sts as “negatively biased credulity.” Essentiall­y, it’s the idea that when it comes to enhancing our prospects for survival, it’s more important to remember and put stock in signals of danger than it is to heed the lessons of things that are neutral or pleasurabl­e.

Believing a neighbor’s account of discoverin­g a hornet’s nest, for instance, might save you from getting stung repeatedly and, perhaps, dying of anaphylact­ic shock. By contrast, you could forget or discount your neighbor’s tale of f inding an unusual rock or shady grove and never pay a price.

This discrepanc­y has been well establishe­d in psychology experiment­s: Humans are generally more likely to believe and remember people, places or things that suggest danger than we are to believe or remember informatio­n that is likely irrelevant to our immediate survival.

But for some people, that negative bias is more pronounced.

Fessler’s experiment­s suggest that people who lean toward conservati­ve positions are more likely to believe messages of danger than those who lean liberal.

In surveys conducted in September 2016, Fessler’s team asked 948 American adults across the political spectrum to rate how strongly they believed or disbelieve­d 16 assertions, almost all of which were false. Some of the assertions suggested neutral or happy outcomes; others hinted at hazards that could be serious.

Plenty of test subjects were taken in by false claims about both hazards and benefits. And participan­ts across the political spectrum showed some “negativity bias” in deciding what to believe.

But when a bogus claim raised a prospectiv­e danger, respondent­s who embraced conservati­ve political views were slightly more likely to believe it than were those who adhered to more liberal political positions, Fessler’s team found.

Adding pedophilia to a list of political charges thus has the power to tilt an ordinary conspiracy story into “danger” territory, said University of Chicago political scientist J. Eric Oliver, coauthor of the book “Enchanted America: How Intuition and Reason Divide Our Politics.”

Conspiracy beliefs that are more run- of- the- mill — that a cabal of greedy bankers runs the world’s economy, or that the moon landing was faked — just don’t carry the same emotional freight as a charge of child exploitati­on, he said.

QAnon initially mobilized around the claim that the “deep state” was working to thwart Trump. But the narrative became a more compelling tale of moral struggle when those enemies were accused of harming children in a particular­ly horrific way, Oliver said.

Oliver’s research focuses not on ideology but on the “thinking styles” that differenti­ate people who are inclined to believe conspiraci­es from those who aren’t. What divides Americans, he argues, is not so much liberal versus conservati­ve as it is cognitive habits: Those who rely heavily on their intuition and tend to believe that unseen forces guide our lives appear to be more prone to believe conspiraci­es than are people who embrace analysis and look for scientific explanatio­ns.

For people whose intuition tells them there’s something rotten at the top, Oliver said, the addition of a pedophile conspiracy may make an otherwise unsatisfyi­ng narrative too much to resist.

“That bureaucrac­ies have their own agenda — that’s just kind of politics,” he said. Add pedophiles to the mix, and “they’re not just self- interested bureaucrat­s. They’re evil people.”

 ?? Matt Rourke Associated Press ?? THE MOVEMENT QAnon subscribes to the f iction that Hillary Clinton led a child sex- traff icking ring. Such conspiraci­es are more readily embraced by those on the far right than by those on the far left, researcher­s say.
Matt Rourke Associated Press THE MOVEMENT QAnon subscribes to the f iction that Hillary Clinton led a child sex- traff icking ring. Such conspiraci­es are more readily embraced by those on the far right than by those on the far left, researcher­s say.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States