San Francisco Chronicle

Picture a leader. Is she a woman?

- By Heather Murphy Heather Murphy is a New York Times writer.

A series of images emerged from a simple prompt: “Draw an effective leader.”

Tina Kiefer, a professor of organizati­onal behavior at the University of Warwick in the United Kingdom, fell upon the exercise accidental­ly, while leading a workshop full of executives who did not speak much English. Since then it has been adopted by organizati­onal psychologi­sts across the world.

Both men and women almost always draw men.

“Even when the drawings are gender neutral,” which is uncommon, Kiefer said in an email, “the majority of groups present the drawing using language that indicates male (he) rather than neutral or female.”

And yet, her clients often insisted that what they meant by “he” is actually “both.”

Several researcher­s in organizati­onal psychology who have had a similar experience with this exercise decided to investigat­e further. How might holding unconsciou­s assumption­s about gender affect people’s abilities to recognize emerging leadership? What they found, in a study posted by the Academy of Management Journal, seems to confirm what many women have long suspected: Getting noticed as a leader in the workplace is more difficult for women than for men. Even when a man and a woman were reading the same words off a script, only the man’s leadership potential was recognized.

The authors focused on one particular act associated with leadership: speaking up with ideas to move a team or company forward.

In one experiment, participan­ts were asked to call into a monthly sales team meeting of a fictional insurance company, during which they would hear from either an Eric or an Erica. Later they were asked to rate the speaker on the degree to which he or she had “exhibited leadership,” “influenced the team” or “assumed a leadership role.”

The Erics who spoke up with change-oriented ideas were far more likely to be identified as leaders than Erics who simply critiqued their team’s performanc­e. But Ericas did not receive a boost in status from sharing the same ideas.

In a second experiment, the researcher­s attempted to investigat­e the benefits of speaking up in the “wild.” They asked participan­ts in a 36-team skills competitio­n at the U.S. Military Academy at West Point to fill out a series of questionna­ires before, during and after their competitio­n.

The authors acknowledg­ed that there were limitation­s in studying this male-dominated place, but the nature of the competitio­n allowed them to control for athletic ability, cognitive ability and other factors.

Still, after the competitio­n, when participan­ts ranked who they wanted to be the team leader, only the men who had spoken up with ideas received a boost.

“It didn’t matter whether women spoke up 1) almost never, 2) rarely, 3) sometimes, 4) often, or 5) almost always,” Kyle Emich, a professor at Alfred Lerner College of Business and Economics at the University of Delaware, and one of the authors, wrote in an email. “Women did not gain status for speaking up, and subsequent­ly were less likely (much less) to be considered leaders.”

A negative response to female assertiven­ess — known as the “backlash effect” — has been documented in other studies on gender and leadership.

“I anticipate­d that we would find that effect,” said Elizabeth McClean, a professor at the University of Arizona’s Eller College of Management and an author of the studies. “But we didn’t.”

So what’s going on here? The researcher­s believe that results may be related to the drawings.

“People have these prototypes in their head about what a leader looks like,” McClean said. “When we see an individual, we ask, ‘Do they fit that?’ ”

If they don’t — even if they are acting like a leader — it’s harder to identify them as one.

This disconnect does not require assuming that “women are less competent than men in general (e.g., intelligen­t, smart, organized, levelheade­d),” Alice Eagly, a psychologi­st at Northweste­rn University and author of “Through the Labyrinth: The Truth About How Women Become Leaders,” wrote in an email. Recently, she has found that many people “tend to credit these qualities more to women than men.”

It’s that the capacity to “take charge,” which is strongly associated with one’s ability to lead, continues to be considered a largely male characteri­stic, she said.

But Erica was taking charge. Why didn’t anyone see that?

When we “process informatio­n through the lens of stereotype,” our interpreta­tion may be “consistent with stereotype­d expectatio­ns rather than objective reality,” said Nilanjana Dasgupta, a professor of Psychologi­cal & Brain Sciences at the University of Massachuse­tts at Amherst.

When people are consistent­ly exposed to leaders who fit one profile, they will be more likely to notice leaders who fit that same profile in the future. That’s how the self-reinforcin­g “confirmati­on bias” cycle works, she said. She added that she would be interested in seeing similar studies on the role of race, something these studies did not investigat­e.

How can this problem be overcome? One reliable way to help people to see more women as leader-like, she has found, is to expose them to more women in actual leadership positions.

 ?? Tina Kiefer ?? Professor Tina Kiefer gets drawings like this one when she prompts people to to draw an effective leader. Both men and women almost always draw men.
Tina Kiefer Professor Tina Kiefer gets drawings like this one when she prompts people to to draw an effective leader. Both men and women almost always draw men.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States