The Atlanta Journal-Constitution

Is this dog dangerous? Shelters struggle to decide

Behavior evaluation­s are giving dogs a live-or-die test.

- By Jan Hoffman

INDIANAPOL­IS — Bacon, a creamcolor­ed retriever mix, took a behavior test recently at an animal shelter here. He flunked.

Bounding into the evaluation room, Bacon seemed like an affable goofball, ready for adoption. But as he gulped down food, Dr. Sara Bennett, a veterinary behavioris­t, stuck a fake plastic hand attached to a pole into his bowl and tugged it away. Instantly, Bacon lunged at the hand, chomping down on it hard.

Shelters have used this exercise and others for some 20 years to assess whether a dog is safe enough to be placed with a family. For dogs, the results can mean life or death.

“If you failed aggression testing, you did not pass go,” said Mary Martin, the new director of Maricopa County animal shelter in Phoenix, which takes in 34,000 dogs annually. Between January and June 2016, 536 dogs were euthanized for behavior, most because of test results.

But now researcher­s, including some developers of the tests, are concluding that they are unreliable predictors of whether a dog

will be aggressive in a home. Shelters are wrestling with whether to abandon behavior testing altogether in their work to match dogs with adopters and determine which may be too dangerous to be released.

In January, Martin stopped the testing. By late June, only 31 dogs had been euthanized for aggression, based on owner reports and staff observatio­ns.

“The tests are artificial and contrived,” said Dr. Gary J. Patronek, an adjunct professor at the veterinary medicine school at Tufts, who roiled the shelter world last

summer when he published an analysis concluding that the tests have no more positive predictive value for aggression than a coin toss.

“During the most stressful time of a dog’s life, you’re exposing it to deliberate attempts to provoke a reaction,” Patronek said. “And then the dog does something it wouldn’t do in a family situation. So you euthanize it?”

The debate over how dogs should be evaluated arrives as efforts to generally improve out-

comes for shelter animals are on an upswing. According to the American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, annual adoption rates have risen nearly 20 percent since 2011 — a period during which owning a “rescue dog” acquired something of a righteous hipness. Euthanasia rates are down, although the ASPCA said 670,000 dogs are put to death each year. Some veterinary schools even offer shelter-medicine specializa­tions.

Shelters are helped by a burgeoning network of rescue groups. They shuttle dogs from high-kill shelters, usually in the South and Southern California, often to foster homes and adopters in the Northeast and Northwest, where spaying and neutering campaigns have reduced puppy availabili­ty.

It is impossible to know how many euthanized dogs scored false positives on behavior testing. Though rare, false negatives also can occur and have proved tragic. In December, workers at Animal Care Centers of New York City saw nothing remarkable on a standard behavior test of a dog named Blue, but noted that he had been surrendere­d for biting a child. A rescue group retrieved him. Blue eventually wound up in a retraining center in Virginia. On May 31, he was finally adopted; hours later, he attacked and killed a 90-year-old woman.

Some high-volume shelters cannot afford time for evaluation­s, much less daily walks for dogs; others have begun de-emphasizin­g their significan­ce. Even Emily Weiss, the ASPCA researcher whose behavior assessment is one of the best-known, has stepped away from food-bowl tests, saying that 2016 research showed that programs that omit them “do not experience an increase in bites in the shelter or in adoptive homes.”

In the surge to modernize shelters, tests were an attempt to standardiz­e measuremen­ts of a dog’s behavior. But evaluation­s often became culling tools. With overcrowdi­ng a severe problem and euthanasia the starkest solution, shelter workers saw testing as an objective way to make heartbreak­ing decisions. Testing seemed to offer shelters both a shield from liability and a cloak of moral responsibi­lity.

“We thought we had the magic bullet,” said Aimee Sadler, a shelter consultant. “’Let’s let Lassie live and let Cujo go.’ From a human perspectiv­e, what a relief.”

The 10- to 20-minute tests, developed by behavioris­ts and tweaked by practition­ers, ask two basic questions: Will the dog attack humans? What about other dogs?

Evaluators may observe the dog react to a large doll (a toddler surrogate); a hooded human, shaking a cane; an unfamiliar leashed dog or a plush toy dog.

But these tests have never been rigorously validated.

Bennett’s 2012 study of 67 pet dogs, which compared results of two behavior tests with owners’ own reporting, found that in the areas of aggression and fearfulnes­s, the tests showed high percentage­s of false positives and false negatives. A 2015 study of dog-on-dog aggression testing showed that shelter dogs responded more aggressive­ly to a fake dog than a real one.

Janis Bradley of the National Canine Research Council, co-author with Patronek of the analysis published last fall, suggested that shelters should instead devote limited resources to “observing the many interactio­ns that happen between dogs and people in the daily routine of the shelter.”

But Kelley Bollen, a behavioris­t and shelter consultant in Northampto­n, Massachuse­tts, maintained that a careful evaluation can identify potentiall­y problemati­c behaviors. Much depends on the assessor’s skill, she added.

In fact, no qualificat­ions exist for administer­ing evaluation­s. Interpreti­ng dogs, with their diverse dialects and complex body language — wiggling butts, lip-licking, semaphoric ears and tails — often becomes subjective.

The most disputed of the assessment­s is the food test. Research has shown that shelter dogs who guard their food bowls, as Bacon did, do not necessaril­y do so at home.

The exercise purports to evaluate “resource guarding” — how viciously a dog will protect a possession, such as food, toys, people. Common-sense owners wouldn’t grab a dog’s food while it is eating. But shelters worry about children.

Bennett suggested that Bacon’s bite of the fake hand didn’t necessitat­e a draconian outcome. With counseling, she said, a household without youngsters would be fine.

The shelter workers dearly wanted to save Bacon. But they were so overwhelme­d that they did not have the capability to match him appropriat­ely and counsel new owners.

So Bacon remained at the shelter for several weeks, waiting. Finally, Linda’s Camp K-9, an Indiana pet-boarding business that also rescues dogs, took him on. He settled right down and recently was adopted. Linda Candler, the director, placed him in a home without young children, teaching the owners how to feed him so he wouldn’t be set up to fail.

“His potential made him stand out,” Candler said. “Bacon is amazing.”

 ?? PHOTOS BY A J MAST / THE NEW YORK TIMES ?? Dr. Sara Bennett administer­s a behavior test to a Lab mix named Bacon at Indianapol­is Animal Care Services. Bacon had been in a shelter for several weeks before an assessment determined that with counseling, he’d be safe to place in a home without...
PHOTOS BY A J MAST / THE NEW YORK TIMES Dr. Sara Bennett administer­s a behavior test to a Lab mix named Bacon at Indianapol­is Animal Care Services. Bacon had been in a shelter for several weeks before an assessment determined that with counseling, he’d be safe to place in a home without...
 ??  ?? A pit bull mix rests during the behavior test at Indianapol­is Animal Care Services. Researcher­s are concluding that standard methods used at animal shelters to evaluate a dog’s behavior are unreliable predictors of whether a dog will be aggressive in a...
A pit bull mix rests during the behavior test at Indianapol­is Animal Care Services. Researcher­s are concluding that standard methods used at animal shelters to evaluate a dog’s behavior are unreliable predictors of whether a dog will be aggressive in a...
 ?? PHOTOS BY A J MAST / THE NEW YORK TIMES ?? Bacon, a Lab mix, participat­es in a behavior test at Indianapol­is Animal Care Services in Indianapol­is.
PHOTOS BY A J MAST / THE NEW YORK TIMES Bacon, a Lab mix, participat­es in a behavior test at Indianapol­is Animal Care Services in Indianapol­is.
 ??  ?? A pit bull mix participat­es in a behavior test at Indianapol­is Animal Care Services.
A pit bull mix participat­es in a behavior test at Indianapol­is Animal Care Services.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States