The Atlanta Journal-Constitution

Stereotype­s keep girls from STEM work

The nation is still failing to diversify computer science and engineerin­g.

- By Allison Master, Andrew N. Meltzoff and Sapna Cheryan

Stereotype­s about what boys and girls supposedly like aren’t hard to find.

Toy advertisem­ents send signals that science and electronic toys are intended for boys rather than girls. Computer scientists and engineers on television shows and movies are often white men, like the guys on “The Big Bang Theory.”

Policymake­rs, teachers and parents sometimes subscribe to these stereotype­s, too. They might spread them to children.

Efforts to combat these stereotype­s often focus on boys’ and girls’ abilities.

But as researcher­s who specialize in motivation, identity and cognitive developmen­t, we think society has largely overlooked another harmful stereotype. And that is the notion that girls are less interested than boys are in STEM.

In our peer-reviewed research — published in November 2021 in Proceeding­s of the National Academy of Sciences — we found that these stereotype­s about girls’ interest in science, technology, engineerin­g and math — or lack thereof — are fairly widespread among young people today. We also found that these stereotype­s actually have an effect on girls’ motivation and sense of belonging in computer science and engineerin­g.

Gains made

Fields like math are close to having gender parity — that is to say, roughly equal numbers of men and women — and women are actually overrepres­ented in fields like biology among college graduates in the U.S.

Yet, the nation is still failing to diversify computer science and engineerin­g. Only about 1 in 5 degrees in computer science and engineerin­g go to women.

Our research shows that societal stereotype­s linking these fields with boys and men act as a barrier that keeps girls and young women away. There have been many conversati­ons about the harm caused by stereotype­s about natural talent, which assert that men are better than women at STEM. But what might be even more detrimenta­l for girls’ motivation are stereotype­s that men are more interested than women in these activities and careers. These stereotype­s may give girls the sense that they don’t belong.

Children’s perception­s

For our study, our first step was to document whether children and adolescent­s believe these societal stereotype­s. We surveyed 2,277 youths in grades 1-12 in 2017 and 2019 about how interested they think girls and boys are in computer science and engineerin­g. The majority of youths reported that boys were more likely than girls to be interested in these fields. Most youths — 63% — believed that girls are less interested than boys in engineerin­g. Only 9% believed that girls are more interested than boys in engineerin­g. These “interest stereotype­s,” if you will, were endorsed by youths from diverse background­s, including Black, white, Asian and Hispanic youths.

They were endorsed by kids as early as age 6, in first grade.

These beliefs about gendered interests were also more common than stereotype­s about ability, that boys are more talented than girls at these fields.

We also discovered that these interest stereotype­s were linked to worse outcomes for girls. The more that a typical girl in our study believed in these stereotype­s favoring boys, the less motivated she was in computer science and engineerin­g.

Effects on motivation

We also did two laboratory experiment­s using a gold-standard random-assignment design to see whether interest stereotype­s have causal effects on motivation. We told children about two activities they could try. The only difference between the activities was that one activity was linked to a stereotype that girls were less interested than boys in that activity.

The other activity was not linked to such a stereotype. If children preferred one activity over the other, we could infer that the stereotype caused a difference in their preference­s. We found that interest stereotype­s can actually cause girls’ lower motivation for computer science activities.

Only 35% of girls chose the stereotype­d activity over the non-stereotype­d activity. These stereotype­s — which favored boys in this case – weren’t a problem for boys, who showed no preference. There was no gender gap when there was no stereotype — a gender gap only appeared when the activity was stereotype­d.

Dismantlin­g stereotype­s

Why are interest stereotype­s so powerful? Interest stereotype­s may make girls assume: If boys like these fields more than girls, then I won’t like these fields, either. They also send a clear signal about who belongs there. A sense of belonging matters a lot for motivation. The lower the girls’ sense of belonging, the lower their interest.

But what if the stereotype­s are true? On average, girls in the U.S. usually do report being less interested than boys in computer science and engineerin­g.

Whether or not these cultural stereotype­s are currently true, we believe they can create a vicious cycle. Girls might miss out on opportunit­ies because of an assumption that they are not interested or should not be interested in certain STEM fields. Unless adults deliberate­ly send girls a different message about who belongs in computer science and engineerin­g, we as a society discourage girls from trying these activities and discoverin­g that they like them.

But the good news is that the lack of belonging that many girls feel in certain STEM feels is not permanent. On the contrary, we think it can be changed.

There are simple ways to send kids a different message about who likes to do computer science and engineerin­g. Parents and other adults can check their assumption­s about what toys to buy girls for their birthdays or holidays, or what summer camps they should attend. Girls can be shown examples of women like Aicha Evans and Debbie Sterling — who are changing the world through technology.

It’s not enough for girls to realize that they can do computer science and engineerin­g. In order to change the status quo, we think it’s necessary to spread the word that many girls actually want to do these things as well.

Allison Master is assistant professor of education at the University of Houston. Andrew N. Meltzoff is professor of psychology at the University of Washington. Sapna Cheryan is professor of psychology at the University of Washington. This piece originally appeared in The Conversati­on, a nonprofit news source dedicated to unlocking ideas from academia for the public.

 ?? COURTESY ?? Stereotype­s have an effect on girls’ motivation and sense of belonging in computer science and engineerin­g. These societal stereotype­s act as a barrier that keeps girls and young women away, the authors write.
COURTESY Stereotype­s have an effect on girls’ motivation and sense of belonging in computer science and engineerin­g. These societal stereotype­s act as a barrier that keeps girls and young women away, the authors write.
 ?? ?? Andrew N. Meltzoff
Andrew N. Meltzoff
 ?? ?? Sapna Cheryan
Sapna Cheryan
 ?? ?? Allison Master
Allison Master

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States