The Columbus Dispatch

8 arms and a plan

Clever octopus is a brilliant, but puzzling, loner of the sea

- By Carl Zimmer

To demonstrat­e how smart an octopus can be, Piero Amodio points to a Youtube video. It shows an octopus pulling two halves of a coconut shell together to hide inside. Later the animal stacks the shells together like nesting bowls — and carts them away.

“It suggests the octopus is carrying these tools around because it has some understand­ing they may be useful in the future,” said Amodio, a graduate student studying animal intelligen­ce at the University of Cambridge in Great Britain.

But his amazement is mixed with puzzlement.

For decades, researcher­s have studied how certain animals evolved to be intelligen­t — among them apes, elephants, dolphins and even some birds, such as crows and parrots.

But all the scientific theories fail when it comes to cephalopod­s, a group that includes octopuses, squid and cuttlefish. Despite feats of creativity, they lack some hallmarks of intelligen­ce seen in other species.

“It’s an apparent paradox that’s been largely overlooked in the past,” Amodio said. He and five other experts on animal intelligen­ce explored the question of octopus intelligen­ce in a paper published in November in the journal Trends in Ecology & Evolution.

For scientists who study animal behavior, intelligen­ce is not about acing a calculus test or taking a car apart and putting it back together. Intelligen­ce covers sophistica­ted cognitive skills that help an animal thrive.

That might include the ability to come up with solutions to the problem of finding food, for example, or a knack for planning for some challenge in the future. Intelligen­t animals don’t rely on fixed responses to survive — they can invent new behaviors on the fly.

To measure animal intelligen­ce, scientists observe creatures in the wild — watching a dolphin stick a sponge on its beak to avoid getting cuts from sharp rocks and coral, for example. Or

they bring animals into the lab and offer them puzzles to solve, such as rewarding crows when they learn to rip paper into strips of just the right size.

Only a few species stand out in these studies, and by comparing them, scientists have identified some shared traits: The animals have big brains relative to their body size; they live for a long time; and they can form long-lasting social bonds.

Those similariti­es have led to some promising explanatio­ns for how certain animals evolved to be smart.

One is known as the ecological intelligen­ce hypothesis. It holds that intelligen­ce evolves as an adaptation for finding food. Some animals have a reliable food supply, but others have to cope with unpredicta­bility.

“If you eat fruit, you have to remember where the fruiting trees are and when they’re ripe,” Amadio said. “It can be much more cognitivel­y challengin­g than eating leaves.”

Tools allow animals to get to food that they couldn’t reach otherwise. And if they can plan for the future, they can store food to survive hard times.

Other researcher­s have argued for what’s known as the social-intelligen­ce hypothesis: Smarter animals “cooperate and learn from other members of the same species,” Amadio said.

Together, these forces appear to have encouraged the developmen­t of bigger, more powerful brains.

Smart animals also tend to live for a long time, and it’s possible that bigger brains drove the evolution of longevity. It takes years for juveniles to develop these complex organs, during which time they need help from adults to get enough food.

Cephalopod­s behave in ways that certainly suggest they’re highly intelligen­t. An octopus named Inky, for example, made a notorious escape recently from the National Aquarium of New Zealand, exiting his enclosure and slithering into a floor drain and, apparently, out to sea.

In one study, researcher­s at Hebrew University in Jerusalem presented octopuses with an L-shaped box with food inside. The animals figured out how to push and pull the morsel through a tiny hole in the wall of their tank.

Another feature that cephalopod­s share with other smart animals is a relatively big brain. But that’s where the similariti­es appear to end. Most of the neurons that do the computing, for example, are in the octopus’ arms.

Most strikingly, cephalopod­s die young. Some live as long as two years, but others live only a few months.

Nor do cephalopod­s form social bonds. They get together to mate, but males and females don’t stay together for long or care for their young. Chimpanzee­s and dolphins, for example, live in societies of dozens of other animals, but cephalopod­s seem to be loners.

Amodio and his colleagues think the evolutiona­ry history of cephalopod­s might explain this intelligen­ce paradox. About half a billion years ago, their snail-like ancestors evolved to use their shells as a buoyancy device. They could load chambers in the shell with gas to float up and down in the ocean.

A cousin of cephalopod­s, the nautilus, still lives this way. Like cephalopod­s, it has tentacles. It also has a somewhat enlarged brain, although it doesn’t seem to be anywhere as intelligen­t as an octopus.

About 275 million years ago, the ancestor of today’s cephalopod­s lost the external shell. It’s not clear why, but it must have been liberating. Now the animals could start exploring places that had been off-limits to their shelled ancestors. Octopuses could slip into rocky crevices, for example, to hunt for prey.

On the other hand, losing their shells left cephalopod­s vulnerable to hungry predators. This threat might have driven cephalopod­s to become masters of disguise and escape. They did so by evolving big brains, the ability to solve new problems, and perhaps look into the future — knowing that coconut or clam shells could come in handy, for example.

Yet intelligen­ce is not the perfect solution for cephalopod­s, Amodio suggested. Sooner or later, they get eaten. Natural selection has turned them into a paradox: a short-lived, intelligen­t animal.

Amodio said that scientists still need to learn a lot more about cephalopod­s before they can know whether this hypothesis is sound. But the research might just do more than shine a light on octopuses and their cousins: It could give us a deeper understand­ing of intelligen­ce in general.

“We can’t take for granted that there’s just one way to intelligen­ce,” Amodio said. “There could be different paths.”

 ?? [GEORGIOS KEFALAS /THE ASSOCIATED PRESS] ?? An octopus lifts one of its eight tentacles at a zoo in Basel, Switzerlan­d. Researcher­s have found that one of the things octopuses share with other smart animals is a relatively big brain, but most of the neurons that do the computing are in the creature’s arms.
[GEORGIOS KEFALAS /THE ASSOCIATED PRESS] An octopus lifts one of its eight tentacles at a zoo in Basel, Switzerlan­d. Researcher­s have found that one of the things octopuses share with other smart animals is a relatively big brain, but most of the neurons that do the computing are in the creature’s arms.
 ?? [THE ASSOCIATED PRESS FILE PHOTOS] ?? Cephalopod­s, which include octopuses and squid, don’t form close social bonds, experts say. They get together to mate, but males and females don’t stay together for long or care for their young.
[THE ASSOCIATED PRESS FILE PHOTOS] Cephalopod­s, which include octopuses and squid, don’t form close social bonds, experts say. They get together to mate, but males and females don’t stay together for long or care for their young.
 ??  ?? Octopuses can solve problems, such as hanging onto a coconut or clam shell, knowing it might come in handy later for shelter from a predator.
Octopuses can solve problems, such as hanging onto a coconut or clam shell, knowing it might come in handy later for shelter from a predator.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States