The Denver Post

“Parentese” is truly a lingua franca, global study finds

- By Oliver Whang

We’ve all seen it; we’ve all cringed at it; we’ve all done it ourselves: talked to a baby like it was, you know, a baby.

“Ooo, hellooooo baby!” you say, your voice lilting like a rapturousl­y accommodat­ing Walmart employee. Baby is utterly baffled by your unintellig­ible warble and your shamelessl­y doofus grin, but “baby so cuuuuuute!”

Regardless of whether it helps to know it, researcher­s recently determined that this sing-songy baby talk — more technicall­y known as “parentese” — seems to be nearly universal to humans around the world. In the most widerangin­g study of its kind, more than 40 scientists helped to gather and analyze 1,615 voice recordings from 410 parents on six continents, in 18 languages from diverse communitie­s: rural and urban, isolated and cosmopolit­an, internet-savvy and off the grid, from hunter gatherers in Tanzania to urban dwellers in Beijing.

The results, published recently in the journal Nature Human Behavior, showed that in every one of these cultures, the way parents spoke and sang to their infants differed from the way they communicat­ed with adults — and that those difference­s were profoundly similar from group to group.

“We tend to speak in this higher pitch, high variabilit­y, like, ‘Ohh, heeelloo, you’re a baaybee!’” said Courtney Hilton, a psychologi­st at Haskins Laboratori­es at Yale University and a principal author of the study. Cody Moser, a graduate student studying cognitive science at the University of California, Merced, and the other principal author, added: “When people tend to produce lullabies or tend to talk to their infants, they tend to do so in the same way.”

The findings suggest that baby talk and baby song serve a function independen­t of cultural and social forces. They lend a jumping-off point for future baby research and, to some degree, tackle the lack of diverse representa­tion in psychology. To make cross-cultural claims about human behavior requires studies from many different societies. Now, there is a big one.

“I’m probably the author with the most papers on this topic until now, and this is just blowing my stuff away,” said Greg Bryant, a cognitive scientist at the University of California, Los Angeles, who was not associated with the new research. “Everywhere you go in the world, where people are talking to babies, you hear these sounds.”

Sound is used throughout the animal kingdom to convey emotion and signal informatio­n, including incoming danger and sexual attraction. Such sounds display similariti­es between species: A human listener can distinguis­h between happy and sad noises made by animals, from chickadees and alligators to pigs and pandas. So it might not be surprising that human noises also carry a commonly recognizab­le emotional valence.

Scientists have long argued that the sounds humans make with their babies serve a number of important developmen­tal and evolutiona­ry functions. As Samuel Mehr, a psychologi­st and director of The Music Lab at Haskins Laboratori­es who conceived the new study, noted, solitary human babies are “really bad at their job of staying alive.” The strange things we do with our voices when staring at a newborn not only help us survive but also teach language and communicat­ion.

For instance, parentese can help some infants remember words better, and it allows them to piece together sounds with mouth shapes, which gives sense to the chaos around them. Also, lullabies can soothe a crying infant, and a higher-pitched voice can hold their attention better. “You can push air through your vocal tract, create these tones and rhythms, and it’s like giving the baby an analgesic,” Mehr said.

But in making these arguments, scientists, mostly in Western, developed countries, have largely assumed that parents across cultures modify their voices to talk to infants. “That was a risky assumption,” said Casey Lew-williams, a psychologi­st and director of the Baby Lab at Princeton University who did not contribute to the new study. Lew-williams noted that baby talk and song “seems to provide an on-ramp for language learning” but that “there are some cultures where adults don’t talk as often to kids — and where they talk a lot to them.” Theoretica­l consistenc­y, while nice, he said, runs the risk of “washing over the richness and texture of cultures.”

In the new study, the sounds of parentese were found to differ in 11 ways from adult talk and song around the world. Some of these difference­s might seem obvious. For instance, baby talk is higher pitched than adult talk, and baby song is smoother than adult song. But to test whether people have an innate awareness of these difference­s, the researcher­s created a game — Who’s Listening? — that was played online by more than 50,000 people speaking 199 languages from 187 countries. Participan­ts were asked to determine whether a song or a passage of speech was being addressed to a baby or an adult.

The researcher­s found that listeners were able to tell with about 70% accuracy when the sounds were aimed at babies, even when they were totally unfamiliar with the language and culture of the person making them. “The style of the music was different, but the vibe of it, for lack of a scientific term, felt the same,” said Caitlyn Placek, an anthropolo­gist at Ball State University who helped to collect recordings from the Jenu Kuruba, a tribe in India. “The essence is there.”

But the jury is still out as to how these cross-cultural similariti­es fit into existing theories of developmen­t. “The field going forward will have to figure out which of the things in this laundry list are important for language-learning,” Lew-williams said. “And that’s why this kind of work is so cool — it can spread.”

Mehr concurred. “Part of being a psychologi­st is to step back and look at just how weird and incredible we are,” he said.

 ?? Manvir Singh, via © The New York Times Co. ?? Manvir Singh, an anthropolo­gist and an author on the new study, records speech in southern Siberut, Indonesia, in 2017.
Manvir Singh, via © The New York Times Co. Manvir Singh, an anthropolo­gist and an author on the new study, records speech in southern Siberut, Indonesia, in 2017.
 ?? Luke Glowacki, via © The New York Times Co. ?? A Toposa woman sings while holding her baby in South Sudan in 2017.
Luke Glowacki, via © The New York Times Co. A Toposa woman sings while holding her baby in South Sudan in 2017.
 ?? Anand Siddalah, via © The New York Times Co. ?? A young member of the Jenu Kuruba tribe in southern India.
Anand Siddalah, via © The New York Times Co. A young member of the Jenu Kuruba tribe in southern India.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States