The News Herald (Willoughby, OH)

When it comes to scientific evidence, conservati­ves value personal stories

- Michelle Sarraf California State Polytechni­c University, Pomona

The Research Brief is a short take about interestin­g academic work.

The big idea

Conservati­ves tend to see expert evidence and personal experience as more equally legitimate than liberals, who put a lot more weight on the scientific perspectiv­e, according to our new study published in the journal Political Psychology.

Our findings add nuance to a common claim that conservati­ves want to hear “both sides” of arguments, even for settled science that’s not really up for debate.

We asked 913 American adults to read an excerpt from an article debunking a common misconcept­ion, such as the existence of “lucky streaks” in games of chance. The article quoted a scientist explaining why people hold the misconcept­ion – for instance, people tend to see patterns in random data. The article also included a dissenting voice that drew from personal experience – such as someone claiming to have seen lucky streaks firsthand.

Our participan­ts read one of two versions of the article. One version presented the dissenting voice as a quote from someone with relevant profession­al experience but no scientific expertise, such as a casino manager.

In the other version, the dissenting opinion was a comment at the bottom from a random previous participan­t in our study who also disagreed with the scientist but had no clearly relevant expertise – analogous to a random poster in the comment section of an online article.

Though both liberals and conservati­ves tended to see the researcher as more legitimate overall, conservati­ves see less of a difference in legitimacy between the expert and the dissenter.

Why it matters

Looking at both our studies together, while about threequart­ers of liberals rated the researcher as more legitimate, just over half of conservati­ves did.

Additional­ly, about twothirds of those who favored the anecdotal voice were conservati­ve. Our data also showed that conservati­ves’ tendency to trust their intuitions accounted for the ideologica­l split.

Other studies of a scientific ideologica­l divide have focused on politicize­d issues like climate change, where conservati­ves, who are more likely to oppose regulation, may believe they have something to lose if policies to curb climate change are implemente­d. By using apolitical topics in our studies, we’ve shown that science denial isn’t just a matter of self-interest.

In stripping away political interest, we have revealed something more basic about how conservati­ves and liberals differ in the ways they interact with evidence. Conservati­ves are more likely to see intuitive, direct experience as legitimate. Scientific evidence, then, may become just another viewpoint.

Though we conducted these studies in 2018 before the pandemic, they help explain some of the ideologica­l reactions to it in the U.S.

Among conservati­ves especially, the idea that the pandemic itself is not a major threat can hold as long as there’s personal evidence on offer that supports that view. President Donald Trump’s recovery from COVID-19 and his assertion based on his own experience that the disease is not so bad would have bolstered this belief. Recommenda­tions from researcher­s to wear masks can remain mere suggestion­s so long as the court of public opinion is still undecided.

What other research is being done

Social scientists are already documentin­g ideologica­l reactions to the pandemic that fit our findings. For example, many conservati­ves see the coronaviru­s as less of a threat and are more susceptibl­e to misinforma­tion. They also tend to see preventive efforts as less effective. Our studies suggest these views will continue to proliferat­e as long as anecdotal experience conflicts with scientific expertise.

What’s next

An individual’s understand­ing of scientific evidence depends on more than just his or her political ideology. Basic science literacy also plays a role.

The pandemic has forced people to confront how hard it is to understand the uncertaint­y inherent in many scientific estimates. Even liberals who are initially more sympatheti­c to science informatio­n might find their confidence in public health messages tested if these messages waver and evolve.

As such, we expect future research will focus on how health officials can most effectivel­y communicat­e scientific uncertaint­y to the public.

The pandemic has forced people to confront how hard it is to understand the uncertaint­y inherent in many scientific estimates.

The Conversati­on is an independen­t and nonprofit source of news, analysis and commentary from academic experts.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States