The Oklahoman

A personal connection

Don't know any COVID-19 patients who've died or been in the hospital? That may explain a lot

- By Tom Avril

Seven months into the pandemic, it's no secret that plenty of people still downplay the risks of COVID-19, scoffing at mask-wearing and social distancing.

Politics clearly feeds that mind-set, as suggested by a new Pew Research Center survey in which Republican­s were more likely to say the crisis was overblown. Others may resist restrictio­ns — capacity limits in restaurant­s, say — because their livelihood­s are at stake. And psychologi­sts have warned that COVID-19 fatigue can lead people to be careless about precaution­s.

But a more fundamenta­l reason for the uneven support of COVID-19 prevention measures may lie in simple math: Despite more than 200,000 deaths and 400,000 hospitaliz­ations in the United States (including more than 1,100 deaths and no available ICU beds in Oklahoma City last week), an Inquirer analysis suggests that in much of the country, the typical person knows no one in either of those categories. (Surveys, including another one by Pew, have reached similar conclusion­s.)

That explanatio­n may seem jarring — do we really need to know someone who is gravely ill in order to practice safe behavior? — but psychologi­sts say it makes sense. Human beings are notoriousl­y poor at evaluating risk, particular­ly when the threat in question is more abstract, or when numbers are involved.

A personal connection to a problem makes it seem more real, said Carnegie Mellon University researcher Julie Downs, director of the Center for Risk Perception and Communicat­ion.

“If you know a bunch of people who have gotten sick, or if you have a family member who died or got sick with COVID — especially if it's somebody you know well enough that you were with them and helping them — you're naturally going to think that it is more likely to happen in general,” she said.

Levels of concern

Other research suggests that a failure to embrace COVID-19 restrictio­ns may be fueled by a lack of empathy, in the same way that someone in rural Pennsylvan­ia may not view urban gun violence as an urgent problem, or that those without military family members may give less thought to the ongoing toll of combat.

In one study, psychologi­sts found that people who expressed higher levels of concern for people with COVID-19 were more likely to report that they had limited social interactio­n to reduce the spread of disease. What's more, the researcher­s found they could influence a person's level of concern.

In the same study, a different group of participan­ts was directed to read a passage about how social distancing would relieve pressure on hospitals and prevent severe illness, especially among the elderly. Another group read the same passage and also watched a video about a 91-year-old man who was worried the virus would infect his chronicall­y sick wife, while a third control group did neither.

Afterward, those who watched the video and read the passage were more likely to express support for COVID-19 restrictio­ns than those who only read the passage, the authors reported in Psychologi­cal Science. And those who only read the passage were no more likely to support restrictio­ns than those who had received no informatio­n at all.

The findings suggest that in seeking to encourage safe behavior, public health officials may need to supplement facts with an appeal to empathy, said lead author Stefan Pfattheich­er, an associate professor of psychology and behavioral sciences at Aarhus University, in Denmark.

“The knowledge is there, but some people don't adhere to this knowledge,” he said. “Maybe informatio­n is not enough.”

It's in the numbers

As for the math of not knowing someone with severe COVID-19, let's take it step by step. To determine the chance that two events will occur, you multiply the probabilit­y of one by the other. Such as when you flip a coin once, the chance of getting heads is 50%. Flip it twice, and the chance of getting heads both times is 50% times 50%— 25%. Three flips, and the chance they all are heads is 25% cubed (12.5%), and so on.

Here's how it works with COVID-19. So far in the United States, one in roughly 800 people has been hospitaliz­ed, meaning the chance that a given person has not been hospitaliz­ed is 799 out of 800 (99.9%) So the chance that any two people have not been hospitaliz­ed is 799/800 times 799 / 800— 99.7%.

For three people, it is 799/800 cubed (99.6%), and so on.

(This method isn't strictly accurate, because picking a person at random from the entire U.S. population is not like flipping a coin, and the odds of COVID-19 hospitaliz­ation for each additional person added to the calculatio­n will be ever-so-slightly different. Also, the true probabilit­y that a given person knows someone with severe COVID-19 will vary widely depending on geography and age, said Ashton Verdery, a demographe­r at Pennsylvan­ia State University. But after hearing a descriptio­n of the Inquirer calculatio­n, he said it was reasonable as a rough approach.)

Then take the fact that the average American knows about 600 people, as researcher­s at Columbia and Princeton Universiti­es have calculated. Take 799/800 raised to the 600th power (multiplied by itself 600 times), and the result is close to 50%— i.e., there is about a 50-50 chance that the “average” person does not know someone who was hospitaliz­ed with COVID-19.

Same goes for deaths. One in about 1,600 Americans has died of COVID-19, leaving 1,599

of 1,600 that have not. Take 1,599/ 1,600 raised to the 600th power, and you find the typical person has greater than a two-thirds chance of not knowing someone who died of the disease.

These estimates are roughly consistent with a Pew survey in August, in which four in 10 Americans said they knew someone who had been hospitaliz­ed or died as a result of having COVID-19. In a reminder that the disease has hit harder in communitie­s of color, 57% of Black Americans said they knew someone who had been hospitaliz­ed or died.

The racial gap is especially acute in Philadelph­ia, according to a new Pew survey last week. Onethird of Black and Hispanic residents said they knew someone who had died of COVID-19, compared with just one out of eight white Philadelph­ians. Human beings have a good sense of relative risk, said Downs, the Carnegie Mellon researcher. We know it is safer to stand back from a cliff than stand on the edge. But when it comes to evaluating numbers, our perception­s are easily distorted.

And after seven months of a pandemic, those numbers will only get bigger.

 ?? [ROBYN BECK/AFP VIA GETTY IMAGES/ TNS] ?? People wearing masks walk on Oct. 2 on a street in downtown Los Angeles, Calif.
[ROBYN BECK/AFP VIA GETTY IMAGES/ TNS] People wearing masks walk on Oct. 2 on a street in downtown Los Angeles, Calif.
 ?? [CHRIS LANDSBERGE­R/ THE OKLAHOMAN] ?? Customers wear masks as they leave Walmart on July 15 in Oklahoma City.
[CHRIS LANDSBERGE­R/ THE OKLAHOMAN] Customers wear masks as they leave Walmart on July 15 in Oklahoma City.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States