The Union Democrat

‘Hard to budge’: Virginia researcher­s are studying COVID-19 vaccine myths

- By KATHERINE HAFNER

NORFOLK, Va. — Many of us spent more time online in the past year after public health officials encouraged us to distance ourselves from each other and stay home during the coronaviru­s pandemic.

It was the safer way to live. But the online world has its dangers related to the virus, too.

The internet has been a breeding ground for misinforma­tion about the pandemic, so much so that the World Health Organizati­on deemed COVID-19 the first “infodemic.”

At Virginia Tech, researcher­s have been studying the phenomenon. With a $25,000 grant from the university’s Fralin Life Sciences Institute, research assistant professor Michelle Rockwell formed an interdisci­plinary team focused on learning more about how misinforma­tion on social media influences people’s plans to receive a COVID-19 vaccine.

Rockwell said she was first interested in social media messaging around health care more generally, “but how could we be interested in anything else (but the vaccine) right now?”

Misinforma­tion is different from disinforma­tion. While the latter is distribute­d purposeful­ly to sow confusion or depress knowledge — think Russian hackers — the former is simply false informatio­n that gets circulated.

The Virginia Tech team decided to focus its study on how misinforma­tion is perceived in the Appalachia­n region, believing that community to be particular­ly vulnerable due in part to historical­ly worse access to quality health care.

The researcher­s designed a social media simulation to see if a post warning people to look out for misinforma­tion would affect people’s perception­s of stories they saw afterward. Rather than a flag — a type of warning label that Facebook, for example, has started adding to posts notifying users to possible misinforma­tion — the warning they used was a social media post itself, discussing misinforma­tion about the vaccine.

The study participan­ts — 1,048 people in the 13-state Appalachia­n region, half in rural areas and half not — saw either a neutral post or a warning post in their social media feed. It was designed to look like it was coming from different sources, including Dr. Anthony Fauci, the chief medical adviser to President Joe Biden; a local hospital; the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; or the individual’s health provider.

After seeing such a warning, people were 40% less likely to rate an erroneous story about the coronaviru­s vaccine as accurate, and 60% less likely to share it, Rockwell said. The researcher­s call that a “nudge” from a trusted health influencer.

The Virginia researcher­s based the misinforma­tion test posts on four COVID-19 vaccine myths they have seen persist online:

• That the vaccine can cause infertilit­y. Almost half of respondent­s to the Virginia study said they believed this. The myth likely originated in a letter sent to the European equivalent of the FDA, according to health news organizati­on STAT. The letter erroneousl­y claimed that the vaccine contains a spike protein called syncytin-1 that is vital for women’s placentas. Coronaviru­s vaccines in fact do not contain syncytin-1, nor do they instruct women’s bodies to generate it. There has been valid debate over whether pregnant women should opt for a vaccine. Medical experts now say they should because infection from COVID-19 can increase risk for complicati­ons that outweigh possible vaccine side effects.

• That there is a high risk of serious side effects, including paralysis. About 40% of respondent­s believed a story about a high percentage of vaccine recipients experienci­ng such side effects. Here’s the truth: The most common side effects are swelling, redness and pain around the site of injection, as well as headache, muscle pain, nausea, fever and chills for a short time following the shot. Women have been reporting more side effects than men, possibly because of their stronger immune response. But extreme effects, like paralysis, are not on the list. There’s also an inherent perception problem, experts say: If someone receives a vaccine and then experience­s a health problem that might have happened anyway, such as a heart attack, people could draw conclusion­s that they were related.

• That Microsoft founder and billionair­e Bill Gates created the vaccine to install microchipp­ed tracking devices in people. About 40% believed this. It’s simply not true. The rumors may have been propelled, as the BBC reported, when Gates said in an interview last year that eventually we “will have some digital certificat­es” showing who had recovered or been tested for the virus. He was referring to the idea of an open-source digital platform to share informatio­n, the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation later clarified. There was never any mention of microchips. The foundation has pledged millions of dollars to efforts fighting the coronaviru­s, particular­ly in low-income countries, but was not involved in developing the vaccines currently on the market.

• That vaccines aren’t real because the coronaviru­s and pandemic aren’t real, either. Less than 20% believed this.

The myths “are sure hard to budge,” Rockwell said.

So how best to combat the infodemic? “There are gobs of informatio­n, and it’s evolving so quickly,” she said. “A very subtle pause and reminder that this (misinforma­tion) is out there, could make such a powerful difference.”

That’s especially true, they found, when the source is your own doctor.

In a survey, the Virginia Tech team found that trust in science and trust in health care in general were the biggest predictors of a person’s readiness to get a COVID-19 vaccine. Younger people and those in more rural areas tended to be less likely to say they’d get a shot.

But when asked who was the most trusted health messenger, or where they’d prefer to get a vaccine if they did, primary care physicians “far and away” topped the list.

As part of the grant, the team is now working with business analytics to track language about the vaccine across Twitter and Reddit through a process called context mapping.

In the meantime, researcher­s hope their early findings can help inform public health officials on how to best get people accurate informatio­n from sources they trust.

 ?? The Virginian-pilot/tns) ?? Stephen M. Katz/
The Virginian-pilot/tns) Stephen M. Katz/

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States